High-Resistance
Interval Training Improves 40-km Time-Trial Performance in Competitive
Cyclists Amy M
Taylor-Mason Sportscience 9, 27-31, 2005 (sportsci.org/jour/05/amt-m.htm)
|
IntroductionIn a review published at this site last
year, Paton and Hopkins (2004) summarized the evidence for beneficial effects
of various kinds of high-intensity resistance and interval training on the
endurance performance of competitive athletes. Although the gains in endurance power
output on average were up to 8%, "all but one study was performed in
non-competitive phases of the athletes’ programs, when there was otherwise
little or no high-intensity training".
They suggested that the gains would probably be less if the high-intensity training
were performed in the competitive phase, when athletes normally include
higher intensity training in their programs. Indeed, in the only pervious
training study performed during the competitive phase of a season (Toussaint
and Vervoorn, 1990), sport-specific resistance training enhanced competitive
time-trial performance of swimmers by an amount equivalent to a useful but
smaller ~2% in power output. In a follow-up study, Paton and Hopkins
(2005) observed improvements of 6-9% in various measures of endurance
power. Evidently, some forms of
resistance training can be very effective, even during a competitive
phase. I was also
interested in the benefits of resistance training for endurance performance,
and coincidentally performed a study on cyclists during the same competitive
season that Paton and Hopkins performed their training study. The outcome is the basis of this
paper. MethodsSubjectsTwenty-four well-trained male cyclists
were recruited through Auckland cycling clubs. All subjects provided informed written
consent in accordance with the University of Auckland human subjects ethics
committee. All subjects in the study
were in the competition phase of their training and were free of injury and
illness. A description of the subject
groups is shown in Table 1. Subjects were randomly assigned in to
either an experimental high-resistance interval-training or a control
normal-training group. Two subjects in
the control group withdrew before the completion of the study. Subjects in the experimental group
performed eight weeks of supervised high resistance interval training twice
per week, in addition to their normal low intensity endurance training. The control group continued with their
normal training programs which was a combination of high intensity racing,
and low intensity endurance training.
All subjects were given detailed training logs to complete four weeks
prior to, and during the eight week intervention period. All subjects repeated the testing procedures
4-10 d following the completion of the intervention. Experimental MeasuresPrior to testing, subjects were instructed
to refrain from intensive training, caffeine, and alcohol for 24 hours, and
to remain on their normal diet. This
investigation was a pre-post design, thus the following procedures were conducted
within one week pre- and one week post-intervention. All testing procedures allowed a minimum of
48 h recovery between tests. On the
first visit to the laboratory, subjects were weighed and sum of eight
skinfolds were measured using skinfold calipers (Holtain, UK). Maximal aerobic capacity (VO2max) was then measured using an incremental
(ramp) protocol with the subject's own racing bicycles mounted on the Kingcycle
ergometer (KingCycle, High Wycombe, UK), which was calibrated prior to each
test. An initial workload of 100 W was
increased 33 W each minute until volitional fatigue. VO2 was measured from analysis of
expired gases (AMETEK OCM-2, Thermox Instruments, Pittsburgh Pa). VO2 was averaged over 20-s intervals. A computer interfaced with the
Kingcycle ergometer measured power throughout the test and peak power was
defined as the highest mean power recorded over any 60-second period of the
incremental test.
Gluteal and quadricep concentric muscle
strength was measured 30 min after the VO2max test using a Biodex
isokinetic dynamometer. The subject’s
hip and knee angles were positioned to simulate top dead centre or the start
of the power phase of the pedaling cycle as described by Faria and Cavanaugh
(1978). The movement involved hip and
knee extension to bottom dead centre or just prior to full knee extension. Maximal repetitions at isokinetic leg
speeds of 180, 270 and 360°.s‑1 (3.1, 4.7 and 6.3 radians.s‑1)
were performed five times and peak torque was recorded as the highest of the
five values. These speeds equate to
30, 45, 60 revs.min‑1 on the bicycle, which represent the range
of cadences used in the high-resistance interval-training program. On a second visit to the laboratory,
subjects performed a 40-km cycling time trial on the Kingcycle
ergometer. To ensure that subjects
gave their maximum effort they were informed that they would receive
incremental financial rewards if they completed the time trial at or above
70% of their individual peak power measured on the first visit and post
intervention incentives based on improvement.
Subjects were permitted to consume fluids ad libitum during the time
trial. Training InterventionCyclists in the high resistance interval
training group performed prolonged rides in the laboratory twice per week,
during which low cadence (40–80 revs.min‑1) intervals were performed as suggested by
Polishuk (1994). All interval training
sessions were supervised by the primary investigator. Sessions consisted of 5-6 intervals of 3 to
22 minutes, and the total interval duration per session increased steadily
from 25 min in Week 1 through 55 min in Week 8. Rest periods in between work
intervals ranged from StatisticsEach dependent variable was analyzed with
a published spreadsheet that used log transformation to estimate the effect
of training as the difference in the mean percent change between the experimental
and control groups (Hopkins, 2003).
Each spreadsheet provided precision of the estimate as 90% confidence
limits and as chances the true effect was practically beneficial and harmful.
For calculation of the chances of benefit and harm, the following values of
smallest worthwhile effects were entered into the spreadsheet for each
variable: 1.5% and 0.65% for 40-km mean power and
time respectively (Paton and Hopkins, 2006); 1.5% for peak power, VO2max, and power-to-weight ratio (on the assumption that changes in
these variables translate directly into changes in mean power in a time
trial); and a standardized mean difference of 0.20 for all other measures
(Hopkins, 2003). Practical
inferences were drawn using the approach described elsewhere in this journal
(Batterham and Hopkins, 2005).
Briefly, if chance of benefit and harm were both >5%, the
true effect was assessed as unclear (could be beneficial or harmful). Otherwise, quantitative chances of benefit
or harm were assessed qualitatively as follows: <1%, almost certainly not;
1-5%, very unlikely; 5-25%, unlikely; 25-75%, possible; 75-95%, likely;
95-99, very likely; >99%, almost certain. Each spreadsheet also
calculated a standard deviation representing individual responses to the
treatment (typical variation about the mean effect from subject to subject)
and another standard deviation representing the typical error of measurement
in the control group between pre and post tests. ResultsThere was little difference in mean characteristics and baseline performance in the two groups (Table 1). The main effect of the intervention period was a substantial enhancement of performance in the 40-km time trial, due mainly to an enhancement in the experimental group and a relatively small impairment in the control group (Table 2). The nett effect on VO2max was similar in magnitude when expressed relative to body mass but a little smaller and unclear when expressed in absolute units. The experimental group also experienced a substantial reduction in skinfold thickness relative to the control group, but changes in body mass and mid-thigh muscle area were more likely to be trivial. The isokinetic testing produced unclear outcomes. Standard errors of measurement for the control group between pre and post tests were: 40-km time-trial time, 1.7%; 40-km time-trial mean power, 4.3%; incremental peak power, 4.8%; VO2max (ml.min‑1.kg‑1), 7.7%; VO2max (L.min‑1), 7.5%; body mass, 1.8%; sum of 8 skinfolds, 11%; mid-thigh muscle area, 2.3%; and peak torques, 8-11%. The 90% confidence limits for the true values of the error of measurement were ´/¸1.5 for all measures. Standard deviations representing individual responses had too much uncertainty for any firm conclusions; for example, the value for 40-km time-trial mean power was 4.4%, but the 90% confidence limits were -5.2% to 8.4%. About half the measures had negative standard deviations for individual responses (owing to greater variation in the change scores in the control group), but the confidence limits all allowed for the possibility of substantial real individual responses.
DiscussionThe main finding of this investigation was that eight weeks of low-cadence high-resistance interval training improved mean power by ~8% in a 40-km time trial in well-trained male cyclists. Furthermore, these improvements occurred during the competition phase of the racing season, when the cyclists were already training and competing at high intensity. The improvements, and those in incremental peak power and VO2max, are similar to those in most previous studies of high-intensity interval and resistance training, when the uncertainty in all the estimates is taken into account. The main difference between the present study and most other previous studies is that the improvements occurred during the competition phase of a racing season, when the athletes were already training and competing at high intensity. Inasmuch as the smallest worthwhile increase in performance for an elite cycling time-trialist is ~1.5% (Paton and Hopkins, 2006), the gains I have observed represent major enhancements. Only two other published studies of effects of high-intensity training on endurance athletes have been performed during a competition phase. The enhancements in my study were greater than the ~2% observed in a study of swimmers (Toussaint and Vervoorn, 1990), possibly because the low-cadence training I achieved with the cyclists was more effective than the protocol devised for the swimmers. The gain I observed in peak incremental power was possibly less than the 6% Paton and Hopkins (2005) observed with cyclists, but their gains in shorter endurance tests (8-9% in 1-km and 4-km time trials) were similar to what I observed in the 40-km time trial. Their resistance-training sets were similar to ours, but they included sets of ballistic jumps. The contribution of the jumps to performance enhancement is unclear. Some of the measures of performance in the present study produced unclear outcomes. The problem appears to have been relatively large errors of measurement for those measures. The errors for 40-km mean power, VO2max, and peak power were twice as large as reported in some reliability studies (Hopkins et al., 2001). The errors in the present study probably reflect real individual variation in performance of the cyclists in the control group over the time frame of the study. There was probably also a substantial contribution of technical (equipment) error to the unacceptably large error of measurement for VO2max. I agree with Hopkins et al. (2001) that measures derived from isokinetic ergometry are too noisy to be useful for tracking changes in performance of athletes. Although my study was aimed primarily at determining the effect of resistance training on endurance performance, I measured several physiological and anthropometric variables that are potentially related to the mechanism of the effect. It is clear that an increase in VO2max could be the main reason for the increase in endurance performance, but I can only speculate that an enhancement of economy was also involved, as in other studies of the effects of resistance training on endurance (Paton and Hopkins, 2004). A contribution from the other component of endurance, fractional utilization of VO2max, is another possibility. An increase in body mass could be harmful for cyclists when the course includes hill climbing. Resistance training can increase body mass by increasing muscle mass, but my training protocol appeared to have little effect on thigh muscle mass, and the only change in body mass was trivial. The loss of skinfold thickness is in principle a benefit, but only if it represents a substantial loss of body mass. Whether the loss of skinfold thickness was a direct effect of resistance training or an indirect effect of a change in diet is unclear. ReferencesBatterham AM, Hopkins WG (2005). Making meaningful inferences about magnitudes. Sportscience 9, 6-12 (sportsci.org/jour/05/ambwgh.htm) Faria IE, Cavanagh PR (1978). The physiology and biomechanics of cycling. New York: Wiley Hopkins WG (2003). A spreadsheet for analysis of straightforward controlled trials. Sportscience 7, sportsci.org/jour/03/wghtrials.htm Hopkins WG, Schabort EJ, Hawley JA (2001). Reliability of power in physical performance tests 31, 211-234 Paton CD, Hopkins WG (2004). Effects of
high-intensity training on performance and physiology of endurance athletes.
Sportscience 8, 25-40 Paton CD, Hopkins WG (2005). Combining
explosive and high-resistance training improves performance in competitive
cyclists. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research 19, 826-830 Paton CD, Hopkins WG (2006). Variation in performance of elite cyclists from race to race. European Journal of Sport Science (in press) Polishuk DA (1994). The means and methods of improving a cyclists strength potential. Fitness & Sports Review International 29, 84-88 Toussaint
HM, Vervoorn K (1990). Effects of high resistance training in the water on competitive
swimmers. International Journal of Sports Medicine 11, 228-233 Published Dec 2005 |